Place Cell
Place-cell firing patterns are often determined by stimuli in the environment such as visual landmarks, and olfactory and vestibular stimuli. Place cells have the ability to suddenly change their firing pattern from one pattern to another, a phenomenon known as remapping. This remapping may occur in either some of the place cells or in all place cells at once. It may be caused by a number of changes, such as in the odor of the environment.
Place cells are thought to play an important role in episodic memory. They contain information about the spatial context a memory took place in. And they seem to perform consolidation by exhibiting replay – the reactivation of the place cells involved in a certain experience at a much faster timescale. Place cells show alterations with age and disease, such as Alzheimer's disease, which may be involved in a decrease of memory function.
The 2014 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine was awarded to John O'Keefe for the discovery of place cells, and to Edvard and May-Britt Moser for the discovery of grid cells.
Background
Place cells were first discovered by John O'Keefe and Jonathan Dostrovsky in 1971 in rats' hippocampuses. They noticed that rats with impairments in their hippocampus performed poorly in spatial tasks, and thus hypothesised that this area must hold some kind of spatial representation of the environment. To test this hypothesis, they developed chronic electrode implants, with which they could record the activity of individual cells extracellularly in the hippocampus. They noted that some of the cells showed activity when a rat was "situated in a particular part of the testing platform facing in a particular direction". These cells would later be called place cells.
In 1976, O'Keefe performed a follow-up study, demonstrating the presence of what they called place units. These units were cells that fired in a particular place in the environment, the place field. They are described as having a low resting firing rate (<1 Hz) when a rat is not in its place field, but a particularly high firing rate, which can be over 100 Hz in some cases, within the place field. Additionally, O'Keefe described six special cells, which he called misplace units, which also fire only in a particular place, but only when the rat performed an additional behaviour, such as sniffing, which was often correlated with the presence of a novel stimulus, or the absence of an expected stimulus. The findings ultimately supported the cognitive map theory, the idea that the hippocampus hold a spatial representation, a cognitive map of the environment.
There has been much debate as to whether hippocampal place cells function depends on landmarks in the environment, on environmental boundaries, or on an interaction between the two. Additionally, not all place cells rely on the same external cues. One important distinction in cues is local and distal, where local cues appear in the immediate vicinity of a subject, whereas distal cues are far away, and act more like landmarks. Individual place cells have been shown to follow either or rely on both. Additionally, the cues on which the place cells rely may depend on previous experience of the subject and the saliency of the cue.
There has also been much debate as to whether hippocampal pyramidal cells truly encode non-spatial information as well as spatial information. According to the cognitive map theory, the hippocampus's primary role is to store spatial information through place cells and the hippocampus was biologically designed to provide a subject with spatial information. Recent findings, such as a study showing that place cells respond to non-spatial dimensions, such as sound frequency, disagree with the cognitive map theory. Instead, they support a new theory saying that the hippocampus has a more general function encoding continuous variables, and location just happens to be one of those variables. This fits in with the idea that the hippocampus has a predictive function.
Relationship to grid cells
It has been proposed that place cells are derivatives of grid cells, pyramidal cells in the entorhinal cortex. This theory suggests that the place fields of the place cells are a combination of several grid cells, which have hexagonal grid-like patterns of activity. The theory has been supported by computational models. The relation may arise through Hebbian learning. But grid cells may perform a more supporting role in the formation of place fields, such as path integration input. Another non-spatial explanation of hippocampal function suggests that the hippocampus performs clustering of inputs to produce representations of the current context – spatial or non-spatial.
Properties
Place fields
Place cells fire in a specific region of an environment, known as a place field. Place fields are roughly analogous to the receptive fields of sensory neurons, in that the firing region corresponds to a region of sensory information in the environment. However, unlike receptive fields, place cells show no topography, meaning that two neighboring cells do not necessarily have neighboring place fields. Place cells fire spikes in bursts at a high frequency inside the place field, but outside of the place field they remain relatively inactive. Place fields are allocentric, meaning that they are defined with respect to the outside world rather than the body. By orienting based on the environment rather than the individual, place fields can work effectively as neural maps of the environment. A typical place cell will have only one or a few place fields in a small laboratory environment. However, in larger environments, place cells have been shown to contain multiple place fields which are usually irregular. Place cells may also show directionality, meaning they will only fire in a certain location when travelling in a particular direction.
Remapping
Remapping refers to the change in the place field characteristics that occurs when a subject experiences a new environment, or the same environment in a new context. This phenomenon was first reported in 1987, and is thought to play a role in the memory function of the hippocampus. There are broadly two types of remapping: global remapping and partial remapping. When global remapping occurs, most or all of the place cells remap, meaning they lose or gain a place field, or their place field changes its location. Partial remapping means that most place fields are unchanged and only a small portion of the place cells remap. Some of the changes to the environment that have been shown to induce remapping include changing the shape or size of the environment, the color of the walls, the smell in the environment, or the relevance of a location to the task at hand.
Phase precession
The firing of place cells is timed in relation to local theta waves, a process termed phase precession. Upon entering a place field, place cells will fire in bursts at a particular point in the phase of the underlying theta waves. However, as an animal progresses through the place field, the firing will happen progressively earlier in the phase. It is thought that this phenomenon increases the accuracy of the place coding, and aids in plasticity, which is required for learning.
Directionality
In some cases place cells show directionality, meaning they will only fire in a location when the subject is travelling in a particular direction. However, they may also be omnidirectional, meaning they fire regardless of the direction the subject. The lack of directionality in some place cells might occur particularly in impoverished environments, whereas in more complicated environments directionality is enhanced. The radial arm maze is one such environment where directionality does occur. In this environment, cells may even have multiple place fields, of which one is strongly directional, while the others are not. In virtual reality corridors, the degree of directionality in the population of place cells is particularly high. The directionality of place cells has been shown to emerge as a result of the animal's behaviour. For example, the receptive fields become skewed when rats travel a linear track in a single direction. Recent theoretical studies suggest that place cells encode a successor representation which maps the current state to the predicted successor states, and that directionality emerges from this formalism. This computational framework also provides an account for the distortion of place fields around obstacles.
Sensory input
Place cells were initially believed to fire in direct relation to simple sensory inputs, but studies have suggested that this may not be the case. Place fields are usually unaffected by large sensory changes, like removing a landmark from an environment, but respond to subtle changes, like a change in color or shape of an object. This suggests that place cells respond to complex stimuli rather than simple individual sensory cues. According to the functional differentation model, sensory information is processed in various cortical structures upstream of the hippocampus before actually reaching the structure, so that the information received by place cells is a compilation, a functional derivative, of different stimuli.
Sensory information received by place cells can be categorized as either metric or contextual information, where metric information corresponds to where place cells should fire and contextual input corresponds to whether or not a place field should fire in a certain environment. Metric sensory information is any kind of spatial input that might indicate a distance between two points. For example, the edges of an environment might signal the size of the overall place field or the distance between two points within a place field. Metric signals can be either linear or directional. Directional inputs provide information about the orientation of a place field, whereas linear inputs essentially form a representational grid. Contextual cues allow established place fields to adapt to minor changes in the environment, such as a change in object color or shape. Metric and contextual inputs are processed together in the entorhinal cortex before reaching the hippocampal place cells. Visuospatial and olfactory inputs are examples of sensory inputs that are utilized by place cells. These types of sensory cues can include both metric and contextual information.
Visuospatial inputs
Spatial cues such as geometric boundaries or orienting landmarks are important examples of metric input. An example is the walls of an environment, which provides information about relative distance and location. Place cells generally rely on set distal cues rather than cues in the immediate proximal environment, though local cues can have a profound impact on local place fields. Visual sensory inputs can also supply important contextual information. A change in color of a specific object or the walls of the environment can affect whether or not a place cell fires in a particular field. Thus, visuospatial sensory information is critical to the formation and recollection of place field.
Olfactory inputs
Although place cells primarily rely on visuospatial input, some studies suggest that olfactory input may also affect the formation and stability of place fields. Olfaction may compensate for a loss of visual information, or even be responsible for the formation of stable place fields in the same way visuospatial cues are. This has been confirmed by a study in a virtual environment that was composed of odor gradients. Change in the olfactory stimulus in an environment may also cause the remapping of place cells.
Vestibular inputs
Stimuli from the vestibular system, such as rotations, can cause changes in place cells firing. After receiving vestibular input some place cells may remap to align with this input, though not all cells will remap and are more reliant on visual cues. Bilateral lesions of the vestibular system in patients may cause abnormal firing of hippocampal place cells as evidenced, in part, by difficulties with spatial tasks such as the radial arm maze and the Morris water navigation task.
Movement inputs
Movement can also be an important spatial cue. Mice use their self-motion information to determine how far and in which direction they have travelled, a process called path integration. This is especially the case in the absence of continuous sensory inputs. For example, in an environment with a lack of visuospatial inputs, an animal might search for the environment edge using touch, and discern location based on the distance of its movement from that edge. Path integration is largely aided by grid cells, which are a type of neuron in the entorhinal cortex that relay information to place cells in the hippocampus. Grid cells establish a grid representation of a location, so that during movement place cells can fire according to their new location while orienting according to the reference grid of their external environment.
Episodic memory
Place cells play an important role in episodic memory. One important aspect of episodic memory is the spatial context in which the event occurred. Hippocampal place cells have stable firing patterns even when cues from a location are removed and specific place fields begin firing when exposed to signals or a subset of signals from a previous location. This suggests that place cells provide the spatial context for a memory by recalling the neural representation of the environment in which the memory occurred. By establishing spatial context, place cells play a role in completing memory patterns. Furthermore, place cells are able to maintain a spatial representation of one location while recalling the neural map of a separate location, effectively differentiating between present experience and past memory. Place cells are therefore considered to demonstrate both pattern completion and pattern separation qualities.
Pattern completion
Pattern completion is the ability to recall an entire memory from a partial or degraded sensory cue. Place cells are able to maintain a stable firing field even after significant signals are removed from a location, suggesting that they can recall a pattern based on only part of the original input. Furthermore, the pattern completion exhibited by place cells is symmetric, because an entire memory can be retrieved from any part of it. For example, in an object-place association memory, spatial context can be used to recall an object and the object can be used to recall the spatial context.
Pattern separation
Pattern separation is the ability to differentiate one memory from other stored memories. Pattern separation begins in the dentate gyrus, a section of the hippocampus involved in memory formation and retrieval. Granule cells in the dentate gyrus process sensory information using competitive learning, and relay a preliminary representation to form place fields. Place fields are extremely specific, as they are capable of remapping and adjusting firing rates in response to subtle sensory signal changes. This specificity is critical for pattern separation, as it distinguishes memories from one another.
Reactivation, replay, and preplay
Place cells often exhibit reactivation outside their place fields. This reactivation has a much faster time scale than the actual experience, and it occurs mostly in the same order in which it was originally experienced, or, more rarely, in reverse. Replay is believed to have a functional role in memory retrieval and memory consolidation. However, when replay is disturbed, it does not necessarily affect place coding, which means it is not essentially for consolidation in all circumstances. The same sequence of activity may occur before the actual experience. This phenomenon, termed preplay, may have a role in prediction and learning.
Model animals
Place cells were first discovered in rats, but place cells and place-like cells have since been found in a number of different animals, including rodents, bats and primates. Additionally, evidence for place cells in humans was found in 2003.
Rodents
Both rats and mice are often used as model animals for place cells research. Rats became especially popular after the development of multiarray electrodes, which allows for the simultaneous recording of a large number of cells. However, mice have the advantage that a larger range of genetic variants are available. Additionally mice can be headfixed, allowing for the use of microscopy techniques to look directly into the brain. Though rats and mice have similar place cells dynamics, mice have smaller place cells, and on the same size track have an increase in number of place fields per cell. Additionally, their replay is weaker compared to the replay in rats.
In addition to rats and mice, place cells have also been found in chinchillas.
Rats furthermore have social place cells, cells which encode the position of other rats. This finding was published in Science at the same time as the report of social place cells in bats.
Bats
Place cells were reported in Egyptian fruit bats for the first time in 2007 by Nachum Ulanovsky and his lab. The place cells in bats have a place field in 3D, which is probably due to the bat flying in three dimensions. The place cells in bats can be based on either vision or echolocation, which remapping taking place when bats switch between the two. Bats also have social place cells; this finding was published in Science at the same time as the report of social place cells in rats.
Primates
Place-related responses have been found in cells of the Japanese macaque and common marmoset, however, whether these are true place cells or spatial view cells is still debated. Spatial view cells respond to locations that are visually explored by eye movement, or the "view of a space", rather than the location of the monkey's body. In the macaque, cells were recorded while the monkey was driving a motorised cab around the experimental room. Additionally, place-related responses have been found macaques while they navigated in a virtual reality. More recently, place cells may have been identified in the hippocampus of freely moving macaques and marmosets.
Disturbances to place cell function
Effects of alcohol
Place cell firing rate decreases dramatically after ethanol exposure, causing reduced spatial sensitivity, which has been hypothesised to be the cause of impairments in spatial procession after alcohol exposure.
Alzheimer's disease
Problems with spatial memory and navigation are thought to be one of the early indications of Alzheimer's disease. Place cells have been shown to degenerate in Alzheimer's mouse models, which causes such problems with spatial memory in these mice. Furthermore, the place cells in these models have unstable representations of space, and cannot learn stable representations for new environments as well as place cells in healthy mice. The hippocampal theta waves, as well as the gamma waves, that influence place cell firing, for example through phase precession, are also affected.
Aging
Place field properties, including the rate of firing and spike characteristics such as width and amplitude of the spikes, are largely similar between young and aged rats in the CA1 hippocampal region. However, while the size of place fields in the hippocampal CA3 region remains the same between young and aged rats, average firing rate in this region is higher in aged rats. Young rats exhibit place field plasticity: when they are moving along a straight path, place fields are activated one after another. When young rats repeatedly traverse the same straight path, connection between place fields are strengthened due to plasticity, causing subsequent place fields to fire more quickly and causing place field expansion, possibly aiding young rats in spatial memory and learning. However, this observed place field expansion and plasticity is decreased in aged rat subjects, possibly reducing their capacity for spatial learning and memory.
This plasticity can be rescued in aged rats by giving them memantine, an antagonist that blocks the NMDA receptors which is known to improve spatial memory, and was therefore used in an attempt to restore place field plasticity in aged subjects. NMDA receptors, which are glutamate receptors, exhibit decreased activity in aged subjects. The application of memantine leads to in increase in place field plasticity in aged rat subjects. Although memantine aids in the encoding process of spatial information in aged rat subjects, it does not help with the retrieval of this information later in time.
Aged rats further show a high instability in their place cells in the CA1 region. When introduced to the same environment several times, the hippocampal map of the environment changed about 30% of the time, suggesting that the place cells are remapping in response to the exact same environment. Contrarily, the CA3 place cells are show increased plasticity in aged subjects. The same place fields in the CA3 region to activate in similar environments, whereas different place fields in young rats would fire in similar environments because they would pick up on subtle differences in these environments. One possible cause of these changes in plasticity may be increased reliance on self-motion cues.
See also
- Spatial view cells, primate hippocampal counterpart for visual field.
- Grid cells
- Head direction cells
- List of distinct cell types in the adult human body
References
- ^ O'Keefe, John (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Clarendon Press. ISBN 978-0198572060.
- ^ Muir, Gary; David K. Bilkey (1 June 2001). "Instability in the Place Field Location of Hippocampal Place Cells after Lesions Centered on the Perirhinal Cortex". The Journal of Neuroscience. 21 (11): 4016–4025. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-11-04016.2001. PMC 6762702. PMID 11356888.
- ^ Jeffery, Kathryn (2007). "Integration of Sensory Inputs to Place Cells: what, where, why, and how?". Hippocampus. 17 (9): 775–785. doi:10.1002/hipo.20322. PMID 17615579. S2CID 3141473. ProQuest 621877128.
- ^ "The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2014". Nobelprize.org. Retrieved 2014-10-06.
- ^ Kiehn, Ole; Forssberg, Hans (2014). "Scientific Background: The Brain's Navigational Place and Grid Cell System" (PDF). Karolinska Institute. Retrieved September 7, 2018.
- ^ O'Keefe, J.; Dostrovsky, J. (November 1971). "The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat". Brain Research. 34 (1): 171–175. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1. ISSN 0006-8993. PMID 5124915.
- ^ Abbott, Alison; Callaway, Ewen (2014-10-09). "Nobel prize for decoding brain's sense of place". Nature News. 514 (7521): 153. Bibcode:2014Natur.514..153A. doi:10.1038/514153a. PMID 25297415.
- ^ O'Keefe, John (1976-01-01). "Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat". Experimental Neurology. 51 (1): 78–109. doi:10.1016/0014-4886(76)90055-8. ISSN 0014-4886. PMID 1261644. S2CID 1113367.
- ^ Eichenbaum, Howard; Dudchenko, Paul; Wood, Emma; Shapiro, Matthew; Tanila, Heikki (1999-06-01). "The Hippocampus, Memory, and Place Cells: Is It Spatial Memory or a Memory Space?". Neuron. 23 (2): 209–226. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80773-4. ISSN 0896-6273. PMID 10399928. S2CID 8518920.
- ^ O'Keefe, John; Nadel, Lynn (1 December 1979). "The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2 (4): 487–533. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00063949. S2CID 144038992. ProQuest 616519952.
- ^ Lew, Adena R. (7 February 2011). "Looking beyond the boundaries: Time to put landmarks back on the cognitive map?". Psychological Bulletin. 137 (3): 484–507. doi:10.1037/a0022315. PMID 21299273.
- ^ Knierim, James J. (2002-07-15). "Dynamic Interactions between Local Surface Cues, Distal Landmarks, and Intrinsic Circuitry in Hippocampal Place Cells". Journal of Neuroscience. 22 (14): 6254–6264. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-14-06254.2002. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6757929. PMID 12122084.
- ^ Etienne, Ariane S.; Jeffery, Kathryn J. (2004). "Path integration in mammals". Hippocampus. 14 (2): 180–192. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.463.1315. doi:10.1002/hipo.10173. ISSN 1098-1063. PMID 15098724. S2CID 1646974.
- ^ Bostock, Elizabeth; Muller, Robert U.; Kubie, John L. (1991). "Experience-dependent modifications of hippocampal place cell firing". Hippocampus. 1 (2): 193–205. doi:10.1002/hipo.450010207. ISSN 1098-1063. PMID 1669293. S2CID 31246290.
- ^ Moser, Edvard I.; Kropff, Emilio; Moser, May-Britt (2008). "Place Cells, Grid Cells, and the Brain's Spatial Representation System". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 31 (1): 69–89. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723. PMID 18284371. S2CID 16036900.
- ^ O'Keefe, John (3 September 1999). "Do hippocampal pyramidal cells signal non-spatial as well as spatial information?". Hippocampus. 9 (4): 352–364. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:4<352::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-1. PMID 10495018. S2CID 1961703.
- ^ Aronov, Dmitriy; Nevers, Rhino; Tank, David W. (2017-03-29). "Mapping of a non-spatial dimension by the hippocampal/entorhinal circuit". Nature. 543 (7647): 719–722. Bibcode:2017Natur.543..719A. doi:10.1038/nature21692. ISSN 0028-0836. PMC 5492514. PMID 28358077.
- ^ Stachenfeld, Kimberly L.; Botvinick, Matthew M.; Gershman, Samuel J. (2017). "The hippocampus as a predictive map" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience. 20 (11): 1643–1653. doi:10.1038/nn.4650. ISSN 1546-1726. PMID 28967910. S2CID 205441266.
- ^ Behrens, Timothy E. J.; Muller, Timothy H.; Whittington, James C. R.; Mark, Shirley; Baram, Alon B.; Stachenfeld, Kimberly L.; Kurth-Nelson, Zeb (2018-10-24). "What Is a Cognitive Map? Organizing Knowledge for Flexible Behavior". Neuron. 100 (2): 490–509. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.002. ISSN 0896-6273. PMID 30359611.
- ^ Bush, Daniel; Barry, Caswell; Burgess, Neil (2014-03-01). "What do grid cells contribute to place cell firing?". Trends in Neurosciences. 37 (3): 136–145. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2013.12.003. ISSN 0166-2236. PMC 3945817. PMID 24485517.
- ^ Mok, Robert M.; Love, Bradley C. (2019-12-12). "A non-spatial account of place and grid cells based on clustering models of concept learning". Nature Communications. 10 (1): 5685. Bibcode:2019NatCo..10.5685M. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-13760-8. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 6908717. PMID 31831749.
- ^ O'Keefe, J; Burgess, N; Donnett, J. G.; Jeffery, K. J.; Maguire, E. A. (1998). "Place cells, navigational accuracy, and the human hippocampus". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 353 (1373): 1333–40. doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0287. PMC 1692339. PMID 9770226.
- ^ Bures J, Fenton AA, Kaminsky Y, Zinyuk L (7 January 1997). "Place cells and place navigation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 94 (1): 343–350. Bibcode:1997PNAS...94..343B. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.1.343. PMC 19339. PMID 8990211.
- ^ Jeffery, Kathryn; Michael Anderson; Robin Hayman; Subhojit Chakraborty (2004). "A proposed architecture for the neural representation of spatial context". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 28 (2): 201–218. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.12.002. PMID 15172764. S2CID 36456584.
- ^ Geva-Sagiv, Maya; Las, Liora; Yovel, Yossi; Ulanovsky, Nachum (2015). "Spatial cognition in bats and rats: from sensory acquisition to multiscale maps and navigation". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 16 (2): 94–108. doi:10.1038/nrn3888. ISSN 1471-0048. PMID 25601780. S2CID 18397443.
- ^ O'Keefe, John (1979-01-01). "A review of the hippocampal place cells". Progress in Neurobiology. 13 (4): 419–439. doi:10.1016/0301-0082(79)90005-4. ISSN 0301-0082. PMID 396576. S2CID 8022838.
- ^ McNaughton, B. L.; Barnes, C. A.; O'Keefe, J. (1983-09-01). "The contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats". Experimental Brain Research. 52 (1): 41–49. doi:10.1007/BF00237147. ISSN 1432-1106. PMID 6628596. S2CID 6193356.
- ^ Colgin, Laura Lee; Moser, Edvard I.; Moser, May-Britt (2008-09-01). "Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping". Trends in Neurosciences. 31 (9): 469–477. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.06.008. ISSN 0166-2236. PMID 18687478. S2CID 17019065.
- ^ Muller, R. U.; Kubie, J. L. (1987). "The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells". The Journal of Neuroscience. 7 (7): 1951–68. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-07-01951.1987. PMC 6568940. PMID 3612226.
- ^ Latuske, Patrick; Kornienko, Olga; Kohler, Laura; Allen, Kevin (2018-01-04). "Hippocampal Remapping and Its Entorhinal Origin". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 11: 253. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00253. ISSN 1662-5153. PMC 5758554. PMID 29354038.
- ^ Anderson, Michael I.; Jeffery, Kathryn J. (2003-10-01). "Heterogeneous Modulation of Place Cell Firing by Changes in Context". Journal of Neuroscience. 23 (26): 8827–8835. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-26-08827.2003. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6740394. PMID 14523083.
- ^ Deadwyler, Sam A.; Breese, Charles R.; Hampson, Robert E. (1989-09-01). "Control of place-cell activity in an open field". Psychobiology. 17 (3): 221–227. doi:10.1007/BF03337772. ISSN 0889-6313.
- ^ O'Keefe, J; Recce, M. L. (1993). "Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm". Hippocampus. 3 (3): 317–30. doi:10.1002/hipo.450030307. PMID 8353611. S2CID 6539236.
- ^ Burgess, Neil; O’Keefe, John (October 2011). "Models of Place and Grid Cell Firing and Theta Rhythmicity". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 21 (5): 734–744. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.07.002. ISSN 0959-4388. PMC 3223517. PMID 21820895.
- ^ Muller, R. U.; Bostock, E.; Taube, J. S.; Kubie, J. L. (1994). "On the directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells". Journal of Neuroscience. 14 (12): 7235–7251. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-12-07235.1994. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6576887. PMID 7996172.
- ^ Dombeck, Daniel A.; Harvey, Christopher D.; Tian, Lin; Looger, Loren L.; Tank, David W. (2010). "Functional imaging of hippocampal place cells at cellular resolution during virtual navigation". Nature Neuroscience. 13 (11): 1433–1440. doi:10.1038/nn.2648. ISSN 1546-1726. PMC 2967725. PMID 20890294.
- ^ Mehta, Mayank R.; Quirk, Michael C.; Wilson, Matthew A. (March 2000). "Experience-Dependent Asymmetric Shape of Hippocampal Receptive Fields". Neuron. 25 (3): 707–715. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81072-7. PMID 10774737.
- ^ Alvernhe, Alice; Save, Etienne; Poucet, Bruno (May 2011). "Local remapping of place cell firing in the Tolman detour task: Place cell firing and detour behavior". European Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (9): 1696–1705. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07653.x. PMID 21395871. S2CID 41211033.
- ^ Jeffery, Kathryn (5 July 2007). "Integration of the Sensory Inputs to Place Cells: What, Where, Why, and How?". Hippocampus. 17 (9): 775–785. doi:10.1002/hipo.20322. PMID 17615579. S2CID 3141473.
- ^ Bourboulou, Romain; Marti, Geoffrey; Michon, François-Xavier; El Feghaly, Elissa; Nouguier, Morgane; Robbe, David; Koenig, Julie; Epsztein, Jerome (2019-03-01). Burgess, Neil; Behrens, Timothy E; Burke, Sara N (eds.). "Dynamic control of hippocampal spatial coding resolution by local visual cues". eLife. 8: e44487. doi:10.7554/eLife.44487. ISSN 2050-084X. PMC 6397000. PMID 30822270.
- ^ Save, Etienne; Ludek Nerad; Bruno Poucet (23 February 2000). "Contribution of multiple sensory information to place field stability in hippocampal place cells". Hippocampus. 10 (1): 64–76. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(2000)10:1<64::AID-HIPO7>3.0.CO;2-Y. PMID 10706218. S2CID 34908637.
- ^ Poucet, Bruno; Save, Etienne; Lenck-Santini, Pierre-Pascal (2011). "Sensory and Memory Properties of Hippocampal Place Cells". Reviews in the Neurosciences. 11 (2–3): 95–112. doi:10.1515/REVNEURO.2000.11.2-3.95. ISSN 2191-0200. PMID 10718148. S2CID 1952601.
- ^ Jeffery, Kathryn J. (2003). The Neurobiology of Spatial Behaviour. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-851524-1.
- ^ Zhang, Sijie; Denise Manahan-Vaughn (5 September 2013). "Spatial Olfactory Learning Contributes to Place Field Formation in the Hippocampus". Cerebral Cortex. 25 (2): 423–432. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht239. PMC 4380081. PMID 24008582.
- ^ Radvansky, Brad; Daniel Dombeck (26 February 2018). "An olfactory virtual reality system for mice". Nature Communications. 9 (1): 839. Bibcode:2018NatCo...9..839R. doi:10.1038/s41467-018-03262-4. PMC 5827522. PMID 29483530.
- ^ Smith, Paul F.; Darlington, Cynthia L.; Zheng, Yiwen (29 April 2009). "Move it or lose it—Is stimulation of the vestibular system necessary for normal spatial memory?". Hippocampus. 20 (1): 36–43. doi:10.1002/hipo.20588. PMID 19405142. S2CID 10344864.
- ^ Jacob, Pierre-Yves; Poucet, Bruno; Liberge, Martine; Save, Etienne; Sargolini, Francesca (2014). "Vestibular control of entorhinal cortex activity in spatial navigation". Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 8: 38. doi:10.3389/fnint.2014.00038. ISSN 1662-5145. PMC 4046575. PMID 24926239.
- ^ Wiener, S. I.; Korshunov, V. A.; Garcia, R.; Berthoz, A. (1995-11-01). "Inertial, substratal and landmark cue control of hippocampal CA1 place cell activity". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 7 (11): 2206–2219. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.1995.tb00642.x. ISSN 0953-816X. PMID 8563970. S2CID 10675209.
- ^ Smith, David; Sheri Mizumori (10 June 2006). "Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory". Hippocampus. 16 (9): 716–729. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.141.1450. doi:10.1002/hipo.20208. PMID 16897724. S2CID 720574.
- ^ Nakazawa, Kazu; Thomas McHugh; Matthew Wilson; Susumu Tonegawa (May 2004). "NMDA Receptors, Place Cells and Hippocampal Spatial Memory". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 5 (5): 368–369. doi:10.1038/nrn1385. PMID 15100719. S2CID 7728258.
- ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (2013). "The mechanisms for pattern completion and pattern separation in the hippocampus". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 7: 74. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00074. PMC 3812781. PMID 24198767.
- ^ Leutgeb, Stefan; Leutgeb, Jill K; Moser, May-Britt; Moser, Edvard I (2005-12-01). "Place cells, spatial maps and the population code for memory". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. Motor systems / Neurobiology of behaviour. 15 (6): 738–746. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.002. ISSN 0959-4388. PMID 16263261. S2CID 9770011.
- ^ Ólafsdóttir, H. Freyja; Bush, Daniel; Barry, Caswell (2018-01-08). "The Role of Hippocampal Replay in Memory and Planning". Current Biology. 28 (1): R37–R50. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.073. ISSN 0960-9822. PMC 5847173. PMID 29316421.
- ^ Joo, Hannah R.; Frank, Loren M. (2018). "The hippocampal sharp wave–ripple in memory retrieval for immediate use and consolidation". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 19 (12): 744–757. doi:10.1038/s41583-018-0077-1. ISSN 1471-0048. PMC 6794196. PMID 30356103.
- ^ Dragoi, George; Tonegawa, Susumu (January 2011). "Preplay of future place cell sequences by hippocampal cellular assemblies". Nature. 469 (7330): 397–401. Bibcode:2011Natur.469..397D. doi:10.1038/nature09633. ISSN 0028-0836. PMC 3104398. PMID 21179088.
- ^ Liu, Kefei; Sibille, Jeremie; Dragoi, George (September 2018). "Generative Predictive Codes by Multiplexed Hippocampal Neuronal Tuplets". Neuron. 99 (6): 1329–1341.e6. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.047. PMID 30146305. S2CID 52092903.
- ^ Buhry, Laure; Azizi, Amir H.; Cheng, Sen (2011). "Reactivation, Replay, and Preplay: How It Might All Fit Together". Neural Plasticity. 2011: 203462. doi:10.1155/2011/203462. ISSN 2090-5904. PMC 3171894. PMID 21918724.
- ^ Geva-Sagiv, Maya; Las, Liora; Yovel, Yossi; Ulanovsky, Nachum (2015). "Spatial cognition in bats and rats: from sensory acquisition to multiscale maps and navigation". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 16 (2): 94–108. doi:10.1038/nrn3888. ISSN 1471-0048. PMID 25601780. S2CID 18397443.
- ^ Hori, Etsuro; Nishio, Yoichi; Kazui, Kenichi; Umeno, Katsumi; Tabuchi, Eiichi; Sasaki, Kazuo; Endo, Shunro; Ono, Taketoshi; Nishijo, Hisao (2005). "Place-related neural responses in the monkey hippocampal formation in a virtual space". Hippocampus. 15 (8): 991–996. doi:10.1002/hipo.20108. ISSN 1098-1063. PMID 16108028. S2CID 35411577.
- ^ Las, Liora; Ulanovsky, Nachum (2014), Derdikman, Dori; Knierim, James J. (eds.), "Hippocampal Neurophysiology Across Species", Space, Time and Memory in the Hippocampal Formation, Springer Vienna, pp. 431–461, doi:10.1007/978-3-7091-1292-2_16, ISBN 978-3-7091-1292-2
- ^ Ekstrom, Arne D.; Kahana, Michael J.; Caplan, Jeremy B.; Fields, Tony A.; Isham, Eve A.; Newman, Ehren L.; Fried, Itzhak (2003-09-11). "Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation". Nature. 425 (6954): 184–188. Bibcode:2003Natur.425..184E. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.408.4443. doi:10.1038/nature01964. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 12968182. S2CID 1673654.
- ^ Wilson, M. A.; McNaughton, B. L. (1993-08-20). "Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space". Science. 261 (5124): 1055–1058. Bibcode:1993Sci...261.1055W. doi:10.1126/science.8351520. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 8351520. S2CID 15611758.
- ^ Mou, Xiang; Cheng, Jingheng; Yu, Yan S. W.; Kee, Sara E.; Ji, Daoyun (2018). "Comparing Mouse and Rat Hippocampal Place Cell Activities and Firing Sequences in the Same Environments". Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 12: 332. doi:10.3389/fncel.2018.00332. ISSN 1662-5102. PMC 6160568. PMID 30297987.
- ^ Dombeck, Daniel A.; Harvey, Christopher D.; Tian, Lin; Looger, Loren L.; Tank, David W. (2010). "Functional imaging of hippocampal place cells at cellular resolution during virtual navigation". Nature Neuroscience. 13 (11): 1433–1440. doi:10.1038/nn.2648. ISSN 1546-1726. PMC 2967725. PMID 20890294.
- ^ Muir, Gary M.; Brown, Joel E.; Carey, John P.; Hirvonen, Timo P.; Santina, Charles C. Della; Minor, Lloyd B.; Taube, Jeffrey S. (2009-11-18). "Disruption of the Head Direction Cell Signal after Occlusion of the Semicircular Canals in the Freely Moving Chinchilla". Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (46): 14521–14533. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3450-09.2009. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 2821030. PMID 19923286.
- ^ Danjo, Teruko; Toyoizumi, Taro; Fujisawa, Shigeyoshi (2018-01-12). "Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus". Science. 359 (6372): 213–218. Bibcode:2018Sci...359..213D. doi:10.1126/science.aao3898. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 29326273.
- ^ Bray, Natasha (2018). "An 'other' kind of place cell". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 19 (3): 122. doi:10.1038/nrn.2018.12. ISSN 1471-0048. PMID 29386614. S2CID 3267792.
- ^ Ulanovsky, Nachum; Moss, Cynthia F. (2007). "Hippocampal cellular and network activity in freely moving echolocating bats". Nature Neuroscience. 10 (2): 224–233. doi:10.1038/nn1829. ISSN 1546-1726. PMID 17220886. S2CID 9181649.
- ^ Abbott, Alison (September 2018). "The Bat Man: Neuroscience on the Fly". Scientific American. Nature Magazine. Retrieved 2020-01-03.
- ^ Yartsev, Michael M.; Ulanovsky, Nachum (2013-04-19). "Representation of Three-Dimensional Space in the Hippocampus of Flying Bats". Science. 340 (6130): 367–372. Bibcode:2013Sci...340..367Y. doi:10.1126/science.1235338. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 23599496. S2CID 21953971.
- ^ Geva-Sagiv, Maya; Romani, Sandro; Las, Liora; Ulanovsky, Nachum (2016). "Hippocampal global remapping for different sensory modalities in flying bats". Nature Neuroscience. 19 (7): 952–958. doi:10.1038/nn.4310. ISSN 1546-1726. PMID 27239936. S2CID 23242606.
- ^ Omer, David B.; Maimon, Shir R.; Las, Liora; Ulanovsky, Nachum (2018-01-12). "Social place-cells in the bat hippocampus". Science. 359 (6372): 218–224. Bibcode:2018Sci...359..218O. doi:10.1126/science.aao3474. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 29326274.
- ^ "Researchers identify 'social place cells' in the brain that respond to the locations of others". phys.org. Retrieved 2020-01-03.
- ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (1999). "Spatial view cells and the representation of place in the primate hippocampus". Hippocampus. 9 (4): 467–480. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:4<467::AID-HIPO13>3.0.CO;2-F. ISSN 1098-1063. PMID 10495028. S2CID 7685147.
- ^ Ono, Taketoshi; Nakamura, Kiyomi; Fukuda, Masaji; Tamura, Ryoi (1991-01-02). "Place recognition responses of neurons in monkey hippocampus". Neuroscience Letters. 121 (1): 194–198. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(91)90683-K. ISSN 0304-3940. PMID 2020375. S2CID 27398046.
- ^ Hazama, Yutaro; Tamura, Ryoi (2019-05-14). "Effects of self-locomotion on the activity of place cells in the hippocampus of a freely behaving monkey". Neuroscience Letters. 701: 32–37. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2019.02.009. ISSN 0304-3940. PMID 30738872. S2CID 72332794.
- ^ Courellis, Hristos S.; Nummela, Samuel U.; Metke, Michael; Diehl, Geoffrey W.; Bussell, Robert; Cauwenberghs, Gert; Miller, Cory T. (2019-12-09). "Spatial encoding in primate hippocampus during free navigation". PLOS Biology. 17 (12): e3000546. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000546. ISSN 1545-7885. PMC 6922474. PMID 31815940.
- ^ White, Aaron M.; Matthews, Douglas B.; Best, Phillip J. (2000). "Ethanol, memory, and hippocampal function: A review of recent findings". Hippocampus. 10 (1): 88–93. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(2000)10:1<88::AID-HIPO10>3.0.CO;2-L. ISSN 1098-1063. PMID 10706220. S2CID 12921247.
- ^ Delpolyi, AR; Rankin, K; Mucke, L; Miller, BL; Gorno-Tempini, ML (4 September 2007). "Spatial cognition and the human navigation network in AD and MCI". Neurology. 69 (10): 1986–1997. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000271376.19515.c6. PMID 17785667. S2CID 23800745.
- ^ Cacucci, Francesca; Yi, Ming; Wills, Thomas J.; Chapman, Paul; O'Keefe, John (2008-06-03). "Place cell firing correlates with memory deficits and amyloid plaque burden in Tg2576 Alzheimer mouse model". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 105 (22): 7863–7868. Bibcode:2008PNAS..105.7863C. doi:10.1073/pnas.0802908105. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 2396558. PMID 18505838.
- ^ Mably, Alexandra J.; Gereke, Brian J.; Jones, Dylan T.; Colgin, Laura Lee (2017). "Impairments in spatial representations and rhythmic coordination of place cells in the 3xTg mouse model of Alzheimer's disease". Hippocampus. 27 (4): 378–392. doi:10.1002/hipo.22697. ISSN 1098-1063. PMID 28032686. S2CID 2904174.
- ^ Zhao, Rong; Fowler, Stephanie W.; Chiang, Angie C. A.; Ji, Daoyun; Jankowsky, Joanna L. (2014). "Impairments in experience-dependent scaling and stability of hippocampal place fields limit spatial learning in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease". Hippocampus. 24 (8): 963–978. doi:10.1002/hipo.22283. ISSN 1098-1063. PMC 4456091. PMID 24752989.
- ^ Burke, Sara N.; Barnes, Carol A. (2006). "Neural plasticity in the ageing brain". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 7 (1): 30–40. doi:10.1038/nrn1809. ISSN 1471-0048. PMID 16371948. S2CID 1784238.
- ^ Schimanski, Lesley, A.; Barnes, Carol A. (6 August 2010). "Neural protein synthesis during aging: effects on plasticity and memory". Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2: 1. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2010.00026. PMC 2928699. PMID 20802800.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
External links
- Media related to Place cells at Wikimedia Commons